CIE 1931 color space

In the study of color perception, one of the first mathematically defined color spaces is the CIE 1931 XYZ color space, created by the International Commission on Illumination (CIE) in 1931.[1][2]

The CIE XYZ color space was derived from a series of experiments done in the late 1920s by W. David Wright[3] and John Guild.[4] Their experimental results were combined into the specification of the CIE RGB color space, from which the CIE XYZ color space was derived.

Contents

Tristimulus values

The human eye has photoreceptors (called cone cells) for medium- and high-brightness color vision, with sensitivity peaks in short (S, 420–440 nm), middle (M, 530–540 nm), and long (L, 560–580 nm) wavelengths (there are also the low-brightness, monochromatic "night-vision" receptors, called rod cells, which have peak sensitivity around 490–495 nm). Thus, in principle, three parameters describe a color sensation. These tristimulus values of a color are the amounts of three primary colors in a three-component additive color model needed to match that test color.

Two light sources, made up of different mixtures of various wavelengths, may appear to be the same color; this effect is called metamerism. And two light sources have the same apparent color to an observer when they have the same tristimulus values, no matter what spectral distributions of light were used to produce them. Thus, red/green/blue values (RGB) are reliable to describe a color.

The problem is that it is not possible to stimulate one type of cone only, because the sensitivity curves overlap. This yields to negative values for color components. To avoid these negative, and to have a component that decribe the perceived brightness, completely theoretical comonents are calculated. These theoritical tristimulus values are given in the CIE 1931 color space, in which they are denoted X, Y, and Z.[5]

Any specific method for associating tristimulus values with each color is called a color space. CIE XYZ, one of many such spaces, is a commonly used standard, and serves as the basis from which many other color spaces are defined.

Meaning of X,Y and Z

In this model, Y means brightness, Z is quasi-equal to blue stimulation, and X is a mix which looks like red sensitivy curve of cones. Thus, XYZ may be confused with RGB cone responses. But, in the CIE XYZ color space, the tristimulus values are not the S, M, and L responses of the human eye, even if X and Z are roughly red and blue. Rather, they may be thought of as 'derived' parameters from the red, green, blue colors.

The CIE standard observer

Due to the distribution of cones in the eye, the tristimulus values depend on the observer's field of view. To eliminate this variable, the CIE defined the standard (colorimetric) observer. Originally this was taken to be the chromatic response of the average human viewing through a 2° angle, due to the belief that the color-sensitive cones resided within a 2° arc of the fovea. Thus the CIE 1931 Standard Observer is also known as the CIE 1931 2° Standard Observer. A more modern but less-used alternative is the CIE 1964 10° Standard Observer, which is derived from the work of Stiles and Burch,[6] and Speranskaya.[7]

For the 10° experiments, the observers were instructed to ignore the central 2° spot. The 1964 Supplementary Standard Observer is recommended for more than about a 4° field of view. Both standard observers are discretized at 5 nm wavelength intervals and distributed by the CIE.[8]

The standard observer is characterized by three color matching functions.

The derivation of the CIE standard observer from color matching experiments is given below, after the description of the CIE RGB space.

Color matching functions

The color matching functions are the numerical description of the chromatic response of the observer (described above).

The CIE has defined a set of three color-matching functions, called \overline{x}(\lambda), \overline{y}(\lambda), and \overline{z}(\lambda), which can be thought of as the spectral sensitivity curves of three linear light detectors that yield the CIE XYZ tristimulus values X, Y, and Z. The tabulated numerical values of these functions are known collectively as the CIE standard observer.[9]

The tristimulus values for a color with a spectral power distribution I(\lambda)\, are given in terms of the standard observer by:

X= \int_0^\infty I(\lambda)\,\overline{x}(\lambda)\,d\lambda
Y= \int_0^\infty I(\lambda)\,\overline{y}(\lambda)\,d\lambda
Z= \int_0^\infty I(\lambda)\,\overline{z}(\lambda)\,d\lambda

where \lambda\, is the wavelength of the equivalent monochromatic light (measured in nanometers).

Other observers, such as for the CIE RGB space or other RGB color spaces, are defined by other sets of three color-matching functions, and lead to tristimulus values in those other spaces.

The values of X, Y, and Z are bounded if the intensity spectrum I(λ) is bounded.

The CIE xy chromaticity diagram and the CIE xyY color space

Since the human eye has three types of color sensors that respond to different ranges of wavelengths, a full plot of all visible colors is a three-dimensional figure. However, the concept of color can be divided into two parts: brightness and chromaticity. For example, the color white is a bright color, while the color grey is considered to be a less bright version of that same white. In other words, the chromaticity of white and grey are the same while their brightness differs.

The CIE XYZ color space was deliberately designed so that the Y parameter was a measure of the brightness or luminance of a color. The chromaticity of a color was then specified by the two derived parameters x and y, two of the three normalized values which are functions of all three tristimulus values X, Y, and Z:

x = \frac{X}{X%2BY%2BZ}
y = \frac{Y}{X%2BY%2BZ}
z = \frac{Z}{X%2BY%2BZ} = 1 - x - y

The derived color space specified by x, y, and Y is known as the CIE xyY color space and is widely used to specify colors in practice.

The X and Z tristimulus values can be calculated back from the chromaticity values x and y and the Y tristimulus value:

X=\frac{Y}{y}x
Z=\frac{Y}{y}(1-x-y)

The figure on the right shows the related chromaticity diagram. The outer curved boundary is the spectral locus, with wavelengths shown in nanometers. Note that the chromaticity diagram is a tool to specify how the human eye will experience light with a given spectrum. It cannot specify colors of objects (or printing inks), since the chromaticity observed while looking at an object depends on the light source as well.

Mathematically, x and y are projective coordinates and the colors of the chromaticity diagram occupy a region of the real projective plane.

The chromaticity diagram illustrates a number of interesting properties of the CIE XYZ color space:

Definition of the CIE XYZ color space

Experimental results: the CIE RGB color space

The CIE RGB color space is one of many RGB color spaces, distinguished by a particular set of monochromatic (single-wavelength) primary colors.

In the 1920s, W. David Wright[3] and John Guild[4] independently conducted a series of experiments on human sight which laid the foundation for the specification of the CIE XYZ color space.

The experiments were conducted by using a circular split screen 2 degrees in size, which is the angular size of the human fovea. On one side of the field a test color was projected and on the other side, an observer-adjustable color was projected. The adjustable color was a mixture of three primary colors, each with fixed chromaticity, but with adjustable brightness.

The observer would alter the brightness of each of the three primary beams until a match to the test color was observed. Not all test colors could be matched using this technique. When this was the case, a variable amount of one of the primaries could be added to the test color, and a match with the remaining two primaries was carried out with the variable color spot. For these cases, the amount of the primary added to the test color was considered to be a negative value. In this way, the entire range of human color perception could be covered. When the test colors were monochromatic, a plot could be made of the amount of each primary used as a function of the wavelength of the test color. These three functions are called the color matching functions for that particular experiment.

Although Wright and Guild's experiments were carried out using various primaries at various intensities, and a number of different observers, all of their results were summarized by the standardized CIE RGB color matching functions \overline{r}(\lambda), \overline{g}(\lambda), and \overline{b}(\lambda), obtained using three monochromatic primaries at standardized wavelengths of 700 nm (red), 546.1 nm (green) and 435.8 nm (blue). The color matching functions are the amounts of primaries needed to match the monochromatic test primary. These functions are shown in the plot on the right (CIE 1931). Note that \overline{r}(\lambda) and \overline{g}(\lambda) are zero at 435.8, \overline{r}(\lambda) and \overline{b}(\lambda) are zero at 546.1 and \overline{g}(\lambda) and \overline{b}(\lambda) are zero at 700 nm, since in these cases the test color is one of the primaries. The primaries with wavelengths 546.1 nm and 435.8 nm were chosen because they are easily reproducible monochromatic lines of a mercury vapor discharge. The 700 nm wavelength, which in 1931 was difficult to reproduce as a monochromatic beam, was chosen because the eye's perception of color is rather unchanging at this wavelength, and therefore small errors in wavelength of this primary would have little effect on the results.

The color matching functions and primaries were settled upon by a CIE special commission after considerable deliberation.[10] The cut-offs at the short- and long-wavelength side of the diagram are chosen somewhat arbitrarily; the human eye can actually see light with wavelengths up to about 810 nm, but with a sensitivity that is many thousand times lower than for green light. These color matching functions define what is known as the "1931 CIE standard observer". Note that rather than specify the brightness of each primary, the curves are normalized to have constant area beneath them. This area is fixed to a particular value by specifying that


\int_0^\infty \overline{r}(\lambda)\,d\lambda=
\int_0^\infty \overline{g}(\lambda)\,d\lambda=
\int_0^\infty \overline{b}(\lambda)\,d\lambda

The resulting normalized color matching functions are then scaled in the r:g:b ratio of 1:4.5907:0.0601 for source luminance and 72.0962:1.3791:1 for source radiant power to reproduce the true color matching functions. By proposing that the primaries be standardized, the CIE established an international system of objective color notation.

Given these scaled color matching functions, the RGB tristimulus values for a color with a spectral power distribution I(\lambda) would then be given by:

R= \int_0^\infty I(\lambda)\,\overline{r}(\lambda)\,d\lambda
G= \int_0^\infty I(\lambda)\,\overline{g}(\lambda)\,d\lambda
B= \int_0^\infty I(\lambda)\,\overline{b}(\lambda)\,d\lambda

These are all inner products and can be thought of as a projection of an infinite-dimensional spectrum to a three-dimensional color. (See also: Hilbert space)

Grassmann's law

One might ask: "Why is it possible that Wright and Guild's results can be summarized using different primaries and different intensities from those actually used?" One might also ask: "What about the case when the test colors being matched are not monochromatic?" The answer to both of these questions lies in the (near) linearity of human color perception. This linearity is expressed in Grassmann's law.

The CIE RGB space can be used to define chromaticity in the usual way: The chromaticity coordinates are r and g where:

r= \frac{R}{R%2BG%2BB},
g= \frac{G}{R%2BG%2BB}.

Construction of the CIE XYZ color space from the Wright–Guild data

Having developed an RGB model of human vision using the CIE RGB matching functions, the members of the special commission wished to develop another color space that would relate to the CIE RGB color space. It was assumed that Grassmann's law held, and the new space would be related to the CIE RGB space by a linear transformation. The new space would be defined in terms of three new color matching functions \overline{x}(\lambda), \overline{y}(\lambda), and \overline{z}(\lambda) as described above. The new color space would be chosen to have the following desirable properties:

  1. The new color matching functions were to be everywhere greater than or equal to zero. In 1931, computations were done by hand or slide rule, and the specification of positive values was a useful computational simplification.
  2. The \overline{y}(\lambda) color matching function would be exactly equal to the photopic luminous efficiency function V(λ) for the "CIE standard photopic observer".[11] The luminance function describes the variation of perceived brightness with wavelength. The fact that the luminance function could be constructed by a linear combination of the RGB color matching functions was not guaranteed by any means but might be expected to be nearly true due to the near-linear nature of human sight. Again, the main reason for this requirement was computational simplification.
  3. For the constant energy white point, it was required that x = y = z = 1/3.
  4. By virtue of the definition of chromaticity and the requirement of positive values of x and y, it can be seen that the gamut of all colors will lie inside the triangle [1,0], [0,0], [0,1]. It was required that the gamut fill this space practically completely.
  5. It was found that the \overline{z}(\lambda) color matching function could be set to zero above 650 nm while remaining within the bounds of experimental error. For computational simplicity, it was specified that this would be so.

In geometrical terms, choosing the new color space amounts to choosing a new triangle in rg chromaticity space. In the figure above-right, the rg chromaticity coordinates are shown on the two axes in black, along with the gamut of the 1931 standard observer. Shown in red are the CIE xy chromaticity axes which were determined by the above requirements. The requirement that the XYZ coordinates be non-negative means that the triangle formed by Cr, Cg, Cb must encompass the entire gamut of the standard observer. The line connecting Cr and Cb is fixed by the requirement that the \overline{y}(\lambda) function be equal to the luminance function. This line is the line of zero luminance, and is called the alychne. The requirement that the \overline{z}(\lambda) function be zero above 650 nm means that the line connecting Cg and Cr must be tangent to the gamut in the region of Kr. This defines the location of point Cr. The requirement that the equal energy point be defined by x = y = 1/3 puts a restriction on the line joining Cb and Cg, and finally, the requirement that the gamut fill the space puts a second restriction on this line to be very close to the gamut in the green region, which specifies the location of Cg and Cb. The above described transformation is a linear transformation from the CIE RGB space to XYZ space. The standardized transformation settled upon by the CIE special commission was as follows:

The numbers below all have the correct number of significant digits per CIE standards.[10]


\begin{bmatrix}X\\Y\\Z\end{bmatrix}=\frac{1}{b_{21}}
\begin{bmatrix}
b_{11}&b_{12}&b_{13}\\
b_{21}&b_{22}&b_{23}\\
b_{31}&b_{32}&b_{33}
\end{bmatrix}
\begin{bmatrix}R\\G\\B\end{bmatrix}=\frac{1}{0.17697}
\begin{bmatrix}
0.49&0.31&0.20\\
0.17697&0.81240&0.01063\\
0.00&0.01&0.99
\end{bmatrix}
\begin{bmatrix}R\\G\\B\end{bmatrix}

The integrals of the XYZ color matching functions must all be equal by requirement 3 above, and this is set by the integral of the photopic luminous efficiency function by requirement 2 above. The tabulated sensitivity curves have a certain amount of arbitrariness in them. The shapes of the individual X, Y and Z sensitivity curves can be measured with a reasonable accuracy. However, the overall luminosity curve (which in fact is a weighted sum of these three curves) is subjective, since it involves asking a test person whether two light sources have the same brightness, even if they are in completely different colors. Along the same lines, the relative magnitudes of the X, Y, and Z curves are arbitrary. One could as well define a valid color space with an X sensitivity curve that has twice the amplitude. This new color space would have a different shape. The sensitivity curves in the CIE 1931 and 1964 XYZ color spaces are scaled to have equal areas under the curves.

See also

References

  1. ^ CIE (1932). Commission internationale de l'Eclairage proceedings, 1931. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. 
  2. ^ Smith, Thomas; Guild, John (1931–32). "The C.I.E. colorimetric standards and their use". Transactions of the Optical Society 33 (3): 73–134. doi:10.1088/1475-4878/33/3/301. 
  3. ^ a b Wright, William David (1928). "A re-determination of the trichromatic coefficients of the spectral colours". Transactions of the Optical Society 30 (4): 141–164. doi:10.1088/1475-4878/30/4/301. 
  4. ^ a b Guild, John (1931). "The colorimetric properties of the spectrum". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London (Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series A, Containing Papers of a Mathematical or Physical Character, Vol. 230) A230: 149–187. JSTOR 91229. 
  5. ^ Hunt, R. W. (1998). Measuring colour (3rd ed.). Fountain Press, England. ISBN 0-86343-387-1. . See pgs. 39-46 for the basis in human eye physiology of three-component color models, and 54-57 for chromaticity coordinates.
  6. ^ Stiles, Walter Stanley & Birch, Jennifer M. (1958). "N.P.L. colour matching investigation: final report". Optica Acta 6: 1–26. 
  7. ^ Speranskaya, N.I. (1959). "Determination of spectrum color co-ordinates for twenty seven normal observers". Optics and Spectroscopy 7: 424–428. 
  8. ^ "CIE Free Documents for Download". http://www.cie.co.at/main/freepubs.html. 
  9. ^ A. C. Harris and I. L. Weatherall (September 1990). "Objective evaluation of colour variation in the sand-burrowing beetle Chaerodes trachyscelides White (Coleoptera: Tenebrionidae) by instrumental determination of CIE LAB values". Journal of the Royal Society of New Zealand (The Royal Society of New Zealand) 20 (3). http://books.google.com/?id=ErI4AAAAIAAJ&pg=PA254&dq=cie+standard-observer. 
  10. ^ a b Fairman H.S., Brill M.H., Hemmendinger H. (February 1997). "How the CIE 1931 Color-Matching Functions Were Derived from the Wright–Guild Data". Color Research and Application 22 (1): 11–23. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1520-6378(199702)22:1<11::AID-COL4>3.0.CO;2-7.  and Fairman H.S., Brill M.H., Hemmendinger H. (August 1998). "Erratum: How the CIE 1931 Color-Matching Functions Were Derived from the Wright–Guild Data". Color Research and Application 23 (4): 259–259. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1520-6378(199808)23:4<259::AID-COL18>3.0.CO;2-7. 
  11. ^ CIE (1926). Commission internationale de l'Eclairage proceedings, 1924. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge.  Note that the 1924 luminous efficiency function seriously underestimates sensitivity at wavelengths below 460 nm, and has been supplemented with newer and more accurate luminosity curves; see Luminosity function#Improvements to the standard.

Further reading

External links